The two aircrafts are skidded for 14.9 s.
To find the answer, we need to know about the Newton's equation of motion.
<h3>What is the Newton's equation that relates velocity, distance, acceleration and time?</h3>
As per Newton's equation of motion
- V²-U²= 2aS
- V= U+at
- V= final velocity, U = initial velocity, S = distance, a= acceleration, t= time
<h3>What's the acceleration of the aircrafts that skidded from 35 m/s after the collision for 112.1 m before achieving 15 m/s?</h3>
- Here, U = 35 m/s, V = 15m/s, S= 112.1 m
- So, 15²- 35²= 2a×112.1= 224.2a
=> a= -1000/224.2= -4.5 m/s²
<h3>What's the time taken to stop if these aircrafts are de-accelerated by 4.5 m/s² from 35m/s?</h3>
- Here, U = 35 m/s, V=15 m/s a= -4.5 m/s²
- 35= 15+(-4.5)t
=> t= 20/4.5 = 4.4 s
Thus, we can conclude that the two aircrafts are skidded for 4.4 s.
Learn more about the Newton's equation of motion here:
brainly.com/question/25545050
#SPJ1
<span>Earth is the only planet able to support _____
Life
</span>
Answer:hhhkkzkxixuhhhdhhdhdhhhdhhhshsbbsbzhhdhhdhhhshhhdhhdusudhggdydyydhshdhgddhsjsudhdhhdh
Explanation:NzjzjhbxxhzhdghsjshshhHjajskajakakakakaiaiaiijhayayayagayahahah
A few different ways to do this:
Way #1:
The current in the series loop is (12 V) / (total resistance) .
(Turns out to be 2 Amperes, but the question isn't asking for that.)
In a series loop, the current is the same at every point, so it's
the same current through each resistor.
The power dissipated by a resistor is (current)² · (resistance),
and the current is the same everywhere in the circuit, so the
smallest resistance will dissipate the least power. That's R1 .
And by the way, it's not "drawing" the most power. It's dissipating it.
Way #2:
Another expression for the power dissipated by a resistance is
(voltage across the resistance)² / (resistance) .
In a series loop, the voltage across each resistor is
[ (individual resistance) / (total resistance ] x battery voltage.
So the power dissipated by each resistor is
(individual resistance)² x [(battery voltage) / (total resistance)²]
This expression is smallest for the smallest individual resistance.
(The other two quantities are the same for each individual resistor.)
So again, the least power is dissipated by the smallest individual resistance.
That's R1 .
Way #3: (Einstein's way)
If we sat back and relaxed for a minute, stared at the ceiling, let our minds
wander, puffed gently on our pipe, and just daydreamed about this question
for a minute or two, we might have easily guessed at the answer.
===> When you wire up a battery and a light bulb in series, the part
that dissipates power, and gets so hot that it radiates heat and light, is
the light bulb (some resistance), not the wire (very small resistance).
Answer:
The evolutionary success of bats is accredited to their ability, as the only mammals, to fly and navigate in darkness by echolocation, thus filling a niche exploited by few other predators. Over 90% of all bat species use echolocation to localize obstacles in their environment by comparing their own high frequency sound pulses with returning echoes. The ability to localize and identify objects without the use of vision allows bats to forage for airborne nocturnal insects, but also for a diverse range of other food types including motionless perched prey or non-animal food items.
The agility and precision with which bats navigate and forage in total darkness, is in large part due to the accuracy and flexibility of their echolocation system. The echolocation clicks of the few echolocating Pteropodidae (Rousettus) are fundamentally different from the echolocation sounds produced in the larynx that we focus on here, and thus not part of this review. Many studies have shown that bats adapt their echolocation calls to a variety of conditions, changing duration and bandwidth of each call and the rate at which calls are emitted in response to changing perceptual demands . In recent years the intensity and directionality of echolocation signals has received increasing research attention and it is becoming evident that these parameters also play a major role in how bats successfully navigate and forage. To perceive an object in its surroundings, a bat must ensonify the object with enough energy to return an audible echo. Hence, the intensity and duration of the emitted signal act together to determine how far away a bat can echolocate an object. Equally important is signal directionality. Bat echolocation calls are directional, i.e., more call energy is focused in the forward direction than to the sides (Simmons, 1969; Shimozawa et al., 1974; Mogensen and Møhl, 1979; Hartley and Suthers, 1987, 1989; Henze and O'Neill, 1991). An object detectable at 2 m directly in front of the bat may not be detected if it is located at the same distance but off to the side. Consequently, at any given echolocation frequency and duration, it is the combination of signal intensity and signal directionality that defines the search volume, i.e., the volume in space where the bat can detect an object.
The aim of this review is to summarize current knowledge about intensity and directionality of bat echolocation calls, and show how both are adapted to habitat and behavioral context. Finally, we discuss the importance of active motor-control to dynamically adjust both signal intensity and directionality to solve the different tasks faced by echolocating bats.
Explanation: